The Story of Competence
The word competence was first used in
linguistics by Chomsky (1965) to distinguish between knowledge of
language in the abstract (competence) and the way in which knowledge is
realized in the production and interpretation of actual utterances
(performance). Chomsky’s idea of competence as knowledge of language
apart from its use was criticized by Hymes (1972), who countered that
not only does competence refer to the individual’s knowledge of the
forms and structures of language, but it also extends to how the
individual uses language in actual social situations. Hymes described
four kinds of knowledge that speakers use in social situations: what is
possible to do with language, what is feasible, what is appropriate, and
what is actually done. This combination of knowledge and use Hymes
called communicative competence, which many people
contrasted with Chomsky’s theory, and the latter came to be known as linguistic competence.
Hymes’s ideas were the basis for an applied linguistic theory
of communicative competence put forward by Canale and Swain (1980), who
related linguistic acts in social situations to underlying knowledge. In
applied linguistics, language testing, and language teaching,
communicative competence was thought of as a characteristic of a single
individual, a complex construct composed of several component parts that
differentiated one individual from others.
Interactional competence (IC) builds on the theories of
competence that preceded it, but it is a very different notion from
communicative competence. Kramsch (1986) wrote that IC presupposes “a
shared internal context or ‘sphere of inter-subjectivity” (pg. 367) and this is
what clearly distinguishes IC from previous theories of competence.
Young (2011) listed the following component parts of IC:
- Identity resources
- Participation framework: the identities of
all participants in an interaction, present or not, official or
unofficial, ratified or unratified, and their footing or identities in
the interaction
- Linguistic resources
- Register: the features of pronunciation, vocabulary, and grammar that typify a practice
- Modes of meaning: the ways in which
participants construct interpersonal, experiential, and textual meanings
in a practice
- Interactional resources
- Speech acts: the selection of acts in a practice and their sequential organization:
- Turn-taking: how participants select the next
speaker and how participants know when to end one turn and when to begin
the next
- Repair: the ways in which participants respond to interactional trouble in a practice
- Boundaries: the opening and closing acts
of a practice that serve to distinguish a given practice from adjacent
talk
IC involves knowledge and employment of these resources in
social contexts. However, the fundamental difference between IC and
communicative competence is that an individual’s knowledge and
employment of these resources is contingent on what other participants
do; that is, IC is distributed across participants and varies across
different interactional practices. And the most fundamental difference
between interactional and communicative competence is that IC is not
what a person knows, it is what a person does together with
others in specific contexts.
Teaching Interactional Competence
Teaching IC might involve two moments. In the first, learners
are guided through conscious, systematic study of the practice, in which
they mindfully abstract, reflect on, and speculate about the
sociocultural context of the practice and the identity, linguistic, and
interactional resources that participants employ in the practice. In the
second moment, learners are guided through participation in the
practice by more experienced participants. There is considerable support
for a pedagogy of conscious and systematic study of interaction in the
work of the Soviet psychologist Gal’perin and his theory of
concept-based instruction. The new practice to be learned is first
brought to the learner’s attention, not in small stages but as a
meaningful whole from the very beginning of instruction.
Concept-Based Instruction
One example of concept-based instruction is the curriculum
designed by Thorne, Reinhardt, and Golombek (2008) to help international
teaching assistants (ITAs) at a U.S. university develop interactional
skills in office hours. The practice they taught was office-hour
interaction between an ITA and an undergraduate student, and they
focused on how ITAs give directions to students. For the first part of
their program, ITAs in training engaged in discussion and activities
that centered on the relation between context and the resources
participants employ in order to construct, reproduce, or resist a
particular practice. They were then exposed to transcriptions of (a)
expert office-hour interactions and (b) office-hour interactions led by
ITAs. They were asked to reflect on the configuration of identity,
verbal, nonverbal, and interactional resources that are employed by TAs
in giving directions to students by discussing four questions about the
transcriptions.
- Who are the participants? What do you think their relationship is?
-
Where do you think the session could be taking place?
-
What is the teacher trying to get the student to do?
-
What language does the teacher use to accomplish this?
In the next step, Thorne et al. (2008) followed Gal’perin’s
suggestion to provide a materialization that represents connections
between the contextual features of the practice and the verbal resources
that participants employ to construct it. The schema for complete
orienting basis of action (SCOBA) that Thorne et al. developed is
represented in Figure 1 to show visually the relationship between
context in office hours and an ITA’s choice of pronoun to direct a
student’s action.
Figure 1. SCOBA of Pronoun Choice in ITA Office-Hour Directives (click to enlarge)

Source: Thorne et al. (2008).
The ITAs used this materialization individually to mediate
cognitive connections between context and language form, which they then
discussed verbally among themselves. The final phase of the
concept-based curriculum was an explicit comparison, which the trainers
provide, of pronoun use in directives in the expert corpus and the ITA
learner corpus (see Table 1). ITA trainees were then asked to discuss
the differences between directives in the expert corpus and directives
in the ITA corpus and offer their explanations for the
differences.
Table 1. Comparison of Pronoun Use in Directives in the Expert Corpus and the ITA Learner Corpus
Directive construction word/phrase |
ITA learner corpus |
Rate per 10k |
Expert corpus |
Rate per 10k |
Ratio of over/underuse |
I suggest OR my suggestion |
30 |
3.35 |
5 |
0.28 |
12.0314 |
You should |
94 |
10.50 |
83 |
4.63 |
2.2710 |
Let’s |
41 |
4.58 |
118 |
6.58 |
0.6967 |
We |
146 |
16.31 |
881 |
49.1 |
0.3308 |
I would |
0 |
0.00 |
64 |
3.57 |
– |
Total words |
89,489 |
|
179,446 |
|
| Source: Thorne et al. (2008).
The advantage that I see of a concept-based approach to
instruction is that a conceptual analysis of a specific practice
encourages portability of the same concepts to other practices in the
domain of academic discourse, whereas in bottom-up or inductive
learning, learners are required to infer general principles from
multiple examples and they must identify a new exemplar as similar to
ones that they have already met. In contrast, a top-down concept-based
approach encourages learners to develop a concept, or theory, of the
domain of instruction. This concept can then mediate their understanding
of other practices in the same domain. In other words, the ITAs
experiencing Thorne et al.’s (2008) concept-based curriculum not only
learn directives in university office hours but can also apply their
theoretical knowledge to other practices in other interactional
practices at U.S. universities.
Conclusion
Interactional competence can be seen as a set of identity,
linguistic, and interactional resources that are distributed among
participants in a specific situation or discursive practice. The
resources include knowledge of the relationships between the forms of
talk chosen by participants and the social contexts in which they are
used. But more than individual knowledge, IC is the construction of a
shared mental context through the collaboration of all interactional
partners. And through concept-based instruction, learners come to
understand that the context of an interaction includes the social,
institutional, political, and historical circumstances that extend
beyond the horizon of a single interaction.
Learners’ development in IC has been reported in longitudinal
studies in which learners’ contributions to discursive practices have
been compared over time. Systematic study by learners of the details of
interaction in specific discursive practices may benefit development of
interactional competence, but we await empirical studies to test that
claim.
In the assessment of interactional competence, several authors
have claimed that a close analysis needs to be made of the identity,
linguistic, and interactional resources employed by participants in an
assessment practice. This interactional architecture of the test may
then be compared with discursive practices outside the testing room in
which the learner wishes to participate. If the configuration of
resources in the two practices is similar, then an argument can be made
to support the generalization of an individual’s test result because the
testee can redeploy resources from one practice to another. Assessing
interactional competence is challenging, however, because IC is locally
contingent and situationally specific, while assessment often requires
comparing language practices across contexts. Future work in the
learning, teaching, and assessment of interactional competence may
resolve this tension.
References
Canale, M., & Swain, M. (1980). Theoretical bases of
communicative approaches to second language teaching and testing. Applied Linguistics, 1(1), 1–47.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hymes, D. (1972). On communicative competence. In J. B. Pride
& J. Holmes (Eds.), Sociolinguistics: Selected
readings (pp. 269–293). Harmondsworth, England:
Penguin.
Kramsch, C. (1986). From language proficiency to interactional
competence. Modern Language Journal, 70,
366–372.
Thorne, S. L., Reinhardt, J., & Golombek, P. (2008).
Mediation as objectification in the development of professional academic
discourse: A corpus-informed curricular innovation. In J. P. Lantolf
& M. E. Poehner (Eds.), Sociocultural theory and the
teaching of second languages (pp. 256–284). London, England:
Equinox.
Young, R. F. (2011). Interactional competence in language
learning, teaching, and testing. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook
of research in second language teaching and learning (Vol. 2,
pp. 426–443). New York, NY: Routledge.
Richard F. Young is professor of English linguistics
and second language acquisition at the University of Wisconsin-Madison.
His recent books include Language and Interaction andDiscursive Practice in Language Learning and
Teaching. |