Arguably the two biggest (and not unrelated) changes in
Theories and Effective Practices in Teaching Vocabulary and Grammar in
recent years are, first, an appreciation of how much the subsystems of
grammar and vocabulary are interdependent, and second, how the patterns
created though their interdependence emerge from use. I treat the former
briefly (because others in this issue treat it more in depth) before
going on to discuss the second change in light of complexity
theory.
For most, the observations of scholars such as Pawley and Syder
(1983), Sinclair (1991), and Nattinger and DeCarrico (1992) drove home
the point that grammar and vocabulary are intertwined. Their
observations, complemented by the rise of, and findings from, corpus
linguistics, have shifted perceptions in the field to the point where
researchers are less likely to think in terms of morphosyntactic rules
generating acceptable sentence structures into which lexical items are
then slotted, and more likely to think that utterances are formed from
the interplay of the two in a lexicogrammar
(Celce-Murcia & Larsen-Freeman, 1999; Halliday, 1994). For
example, few speakers of English can accept any verb other than need in the pattern My house needs
painting or Our washer needs fixing. In
this sentence, then, what can fill the verb slot is highly restricted.
To give another example, if the verb insist is used,
either on or that is very likely
to follow.
These days, many linguists use the term constructions to encompass all lexicogrammatical
forms, ranging from morphemes and syntactic structures to meaningful
phrasal and clausal sequences or patterns (Tomasello, 2003). This change
is accompanied by the awareness that speakers’ language resources
consist of a large repertoire of phrases such as how do you
know, if you wanna, it is obvious that, can be found, what I mean, what
I’m talking about (Simpson-Vlach & Ellis, 2010). In
short, what used to be seen as a homogeneous linguistic competence is in
essence a heterogeneous network of language-using patterns
(Larsen-Freeman & Cameron, 2008) that language users draw upon
and add to as they use language in real time. Clearly, to develop such
language resources takes extensive opportunity to engage with meaningful
language use (Wray 2002). Accelerating the acquisition of such patterns
in classroom instruction takes focused attention (e.g., Lewis, 1993),
aided by the use of corpus-informed syllabi (e.g., O’Keeffe, McCarthy,
& Carter 2007).
But the question that has captured more of my attention has to
do with the origin and evolution of such patterned sequences. Here, I
turn to complexity theory for its insights. Complexity theory sees
language as a complex adaptive system (CAS; Ellis &
Larsen-Freeman, 2009). In such a system, the patterns are not the
product of rules, but rather arise bottom up from use. This position is
aligned with that of cognitive grammar (Langacker, 1987).A similar
sentiment was expressed by Hopper (1988) in his emergent
grammar: Frequently occurring constructions become
“sedimented” patterns. “The notion of Emergent Grammar is meant to
suggest that structure, or regularity, comes out of discourse and is
shaped by discourse in an ongoing process. Grammar is, in this view,
simply the name for certain categories of observed repetitions in
discourse” (Hopper, 1998, p. 156).
As words are used together frequently, the resulting patterns
become less open and more fixed. Through a process of grammaticalization (Hopper & Traugott, 1993),
word combinations transform into grammatical constructions. Bybee
(2006), for instance, offers evidence in support of her claim that the to in the future periphrastic modal be going
to was originally an infinitive marker, followed by a verb.
Over the years, as its use with going became more
frequent, the going and the to
fused, such that today, the reduced form in speech is common, gonna, and a further truncated form /áymənə/ is used
in the first person, I am going to (I am going to go
to town = /áymənə/ go to town). Notice that /áymənə/ cannot be used for
the first person of the present progressive (I’m going to
town) even though the word sequence is the same(*áymənə to
town).
Because this process of change is relentless, even adult
grammars are not static, but instead have potential to change with
experience (The 5 Graces Group in Ellis & Larsen-Freeman, 2009).
While all this talk of change may prove unsettling to those who think
of grammar as a static rule-governed system, notice that a dynamic,
changing view can accommodate what is known to be true: not only that
language changes over time, but also as it does, exceptions to its rules
become commonplace. For instance, it is increasingly common these days
to hear English speakers form comparatives of adjectives with the
periphrastic more rather than the –er inflection, even for monosyllabic adjectives (e.g., Detergent X will get your clothes more white than detergent
Y.). From a complexity theory point of view, “the act of
playing the game has a way of changing the rules” (Gleick 1987, p. 24).
Such observations also suggest that since speakers’ grammars
are constructed out of their experience with language, each speaker’s
grammar is unique. We expect diversity in grammatical judgments, for
example. Of course, there is sufficient overlap for mutual
intelligibility. This comes about because speakers recognize that they
have choices in how they enact their language resources. They
co-adapt—they make adjustments in their language use to accommodate
their interlocutors.
The implications for learning are great. The focus shifts from
the acquisition of static decontextualized structures of linguistic form
to language development in existing communities of practice. Learners
need to draw on their language resources (including their knowledge of
other languages) to realize their communicative needs. They are not
confined to that which has been already realized in the existing system.
They need to exploit the potential of the system to make meaning. Along
the way, from a target language perspective, regress as well as
progress happens.
What are the implications for this view for teaching?
Traditional approaches to teaching assume that language is a static,
finite system and that practice leads in a linear way to increasing
control of such a system. Practice is therefore basically rehearsal.
However, teaching a language does not involve the transmission of a
closed system of knowledge.
Newer views recognize that different learners obtain different
benefits from the same practice activities (Larsen-Freeman, 2006).
Language learning is not about conformity to uniformity (Larsen-Freeman,
2003). Teaching should enable learners to go beyond the input, indeed
beyond any static mental grammar. Doing so, however, requires a certain
type of engagement, one that is psychologically authentic.
Psychologically authentic (which is not the same as linguistically
authentic) practice gives learners an opportunity to use language
meaningfully for their own purposes, where the conditions of practice
and the conditions of use are aligned (Lightbown, 2008; Segalowitz,
2003).
A second implication is that not only language but also
adaptation should be taught. In order to avoid the “inert knowledge
problem” (Whitehead, 1929), students have to learn to use what they know
to adapt to a changing context. One way to give students practice with
adaptation, for example, is to have them repeat a task, where the
conditions they are given for completing the task are altered with each
repetition. Repeating a task is not simple repetition, but rather
involves iteration (Larsen-Freeman, 2011). In a complex system, the
results of one iteration are used as the starting point for the next
iteration. Thus, the starting point is always different (van Geert,
2003). Learning is not adding pieces to the system; it is changing the
system (Feldman, 2006).
In conclusion, newer theories in teaching grammar and
vocabulary regard language not as a single homogeneous system to be
acquired. Rather, it is thought that stable lexicogrammatical forms
emerge and are learned from use. Such a view “foregrounds the centrality
of variation among different speakers and their developing awareness of
the choice they have in how they use patterns within a social context”
(Larsen-Freeman & Cameron, 2008, p. 116). The same view also
endorses the claim that “learning is construed as the development of
increasing effective ways of dealing with the world and its meanings”
(van Lier, 2000, p. 246).
REFERENCES
Bybee, J. (2006). From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to
repetition. Language, 82, 711–733.
Celce-Murcia, M., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (1999). The grammar book (2nd ed.). Boston, MA:
Heinle/Cengage.
Ellis, N., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (Eds.). (2009).
Language as a complex adaptive system. Language Learning,
59(Suppl. 1).
Feldman, J. (2006). From molecule to metaphor. A
neural theory of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gleick, J. (1987). Chaos: Making a new science. New York, NY: Penguin Books.
Halliday, M. A. K. (1994). An introduction to
functional grammar (2nd ed.). London: Edward Arnold.
Hopper, P. (1988). Emergent grammar and the a priori grammar
postulate. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Linguistics in context:
Connecting observation and understanding (pp. 139-157).
Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Hopper, P. (1998). Emergent grammar. In M. Tomasello (Ed.), The new psychology of language (pp. 155-177). Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hopper, P., & Traugott, E.. (1993). Grammaticalization. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press.
Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of cognitive
grammar: Vol. 1. Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2003). Teaching language: From
grammar to grammaring. Boston, MA: Heinle/Cengage.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2006). The emergence of complexity,
fluency, and accuracy in the oral and written production of five Chinese
learners of English. Applied Linguistics, 27,
590–619.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2011, September). Using complexity
theory to understand the use of repetition in language
teaching. A plenary address delivered at the British
Association of Applied Linguistics Conference, Bristol,
England.
Larsen-Freeman, D., & Cameron, L. (2008). Complex systems and applied linguistics. Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Lewis, M. (1993). The lexical approach. Hove, England: Language Teaching Publications.
Lightbown, P. (2008). Transfer appropriate processing as a
model for classroom language acquisition. In Z-H. Han (Ed.), Understanding second language process (pp. 27-45).
Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.
Nattinger, J., & DeCarrico, J. (1992). Lexical
phrases and language teaching. Oxford, England: Oxford
University Press.
O’Keeffe, A., McCarthy, M., & Carter, R. (2007). From corpus to classroom: Language use and language teaching. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Pawley, A., & Syder, F. (1983). Two puzzles for
linguistic theory: Nativelike selection and nativelike fluency. In J.
Richards & R. Schmidt (Eds.), Language and
communication (pp. 191-226). London: Longman.
Segalowitz, N. (2003). Automaticity and second languages. In C.
Doughty & M. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second
language acquisition (pp. 382-408). Malden, MA:
Blackwell.
Simpson-Vlach, R., & Ellis, N. (2010). An academic
formulas list: New methods in phraseology research. Applied
Linguistics, 31, 487-512.
Sinclair, J. (1991). Corpus, concordance and
collocation. Oxford, England: Oxford University
Press.
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language. A
usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
van Geert, P. (2003). Dynamic systems approaches and modeling
of developmental processes. In J. Valsiner & K. Connolly (Eds.),Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 640-672).
London: Sage.
van Lier, L. (2000). From input to affordance:
Social-interactive learning from an ecological perspective. In J.
Lantolf (Ed). Sociocultural Theory and Second Language Learning(pp. 243-259). Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Whitehead, A. N. (1929). The aims of education. New York: Macmillan.
Wray, A. (2002).Formulaic language and the lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Diane Larsen-Freeman is professor of education,
professor of linguistics, and research scientist at the English Language
Institute, and faculty associate at the Center for the Study of Complex
Systems at the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. She is also a
Distinguished Senior Faculty Fellow at the Graduate SIT Institute in
Brattleboro, Vermont. |