Listening in a second language (L2) is the main way in which
learners expand their understanding of spoken input. An essential
element of language learning, therefore, is listening (Field, 2008).
However, when English is read and written more often than it is spoken
or heard, oral communication for some language learners can be a
challenge (Siegel, 2016). Language learners who are in English for
academic purposes (EAP) settings often have fairly developed vocabulary
skills when it comes to words in print, but their awareness of these
words in oral language can pale in comparison to their knowledge of the
written word. To help EAP students recognize the words they have studied
in the stream of input, a more focused approach to L2 listening
pedagogy may be necessary (Siegel, 2016). Moreover, language educators
may benefit from practical approaches, techniques, and activities they
can apply in their classrooms.
Bottom-Up Versus Top-Down Listening Processes
In cognitive linguistics, the process of listening
comprehension is often referred to as top-down vs. bottom-up listening. A
way to conceptualize bottom-up processing is through the lens of an L2
learner who must attend to intonation patterns, word prefixes, or other
linguistic features to make meaning more accessible. An example of
bottom-up processing is understanding each word and detail (i.e.,
attempting to figure out the meanings of most words or sentences in the
input)—what the literacy world calls decoding. When
L2 listeners are decoding acoustic information, the decoding process
starts from the sound elements of the target language, such as phonemes
and syllables, and then progresses into words, phrases, and sentences.
In tandem, listeners also use top-down processes which tap into prior
knowledge and enable listeners to use strategies such as note-taking and
inferencing to make sense of speech.
A more advanced knowledge on this topic includes the notion
that neither of these processes happens in isolation and that they are
rather an interactive process (Field, 2008). The challenge, in our view,
as seasoned practitioners who have taught EAP classes for many years
and conducted our own research, is that often the bottom-up skills are
overlooked in EAP contexts in favor of a more traditional academic
approach, which favors note-taking as the common academic listening
practice. What note-taking presupposes, however, is that the L2 learner
already knows how to make sense of the stream of speech. That is, he or
she can already recognize presumably known words in their aural form and
do something with that knowledge (Siegel, 2016).
A number of studies have investigated the effects of bottom-up
listening processes. For example, Goh (2000) has identified problem
areas in listening perception and parsing, concluding that problems with
perception were greater than parsing. Matthews and Cheng’s (2015)
experiments with word recognition also relate to one of the perception
problems that Goh (2000) identified, which was the inability to
recognize known words in connected speech. Jensen and Vinther’s (2003)
repetition studies further tried to find a solution to these perception
and parsing problems and found favorable evidence for the role of exact
repetition. All of these studies give insight into what an
evidence-based practice that integrates theory into practice looks like.
Our view is that by reconceptualizing our L2 listening pedagogy
to—once again—include these bottom-up processes, L2 listeners can more
consciously and holistically work toward advancement in their L2
listening comprehension skills.
Five Explicit Strategies Identified in the Research
The following explicit strategies are intended to raise
learners’ metacognitive awareness of the speech stream and provide
opportunities for prediction and reflection. Additionally, these
strategies allow learners to rely on their other English language skills
to bolster their listening proficiency.
1. Dictation
Dictation is a useful practice that allows learners to rely on
learned sentence structures and vocabulary. This activity should
emphasize content and raise awareness of function. In this activity,
sentences may be dictated multiple times and should be stated with
standard word stress, sentence stress, intonation, and thought group
pauses. The length and complexity of the sentences is determined by
proficiency level and curricular goals. Grading criteria and/or feedback
for dictation should favor content accuracy but include function
accuracy. For example, if a learner writes survive in
place of survey, that error would carry more weight
than if a learner writes increase in place of increased. This activity can serve as both practice
and assessment.
2. Dictogloss
In a dictogloss activity, learners create their own transcripts
based on a short audio sample. Learners may listen to the sample
multiple times until they feel confident in their transcription. Once
learners have completed their individual transcripts, they compare what
they have written with the actual transcript and mark the differences.
They are directed to note differences in word boundaries, word endings,
content word errors, and function word errors. Learners are then
encouraged to reflect on their errors and any patterns that they notice.
This can be done verbally in a class discussion or in writing.
3. Transcript Analysis
Transcript analysis activities work well as tools for
predicting content and spoken language features. In this way, they can
serve as a bridge between top-down and bottom-up listening strategies.
Predicting Content: Learners are given
segments of a transcript and listen to the corresponding audio. After
reading the transcript and listening to the audio, learners make
predictions about what the speaker will say next based on the content
and verbal cues. Students receive the next segment of the transcript and
listen to the corresponding audio to check their predictions and make
new predictions for the following segment. Learners continue this
process through the entire lecture and discuss their
strategies.
Predicting Features: Learners are introduced
to the following pronunciation features of English and given examples
of each: stress, thought groups, intonation, contractions, linking,
reductions, and assimilation. They are then asked to analyze a partial
transcript and make predictions about where they expect to hear each
feature produced by the speaker. Learners listen, check their
predictions, and discuss the activity.
4. Cloze Listening
Cloze listening tasks, in our experience, are the most commonly
used activities for practicing and assessing bottom-up listening
strategies. These tasks can be incredibly useful in assessing learner
needs and raising learner awareness. In cloze listening tasks, learners
are given a partial transcript with missing tokens (key words, numbers,
etc.). Before the task, learners should make predictions about the
content of each blank (part of speech, plurality, etc.). After the task,
learners should reflect on the phonemic and semantic relationship
between their answers and the target.
5. IPA/Pronunciation Practice
There is a notable lack of bottom-up listening resources in
most EAP textbooks. One way to mitigate this absence is by using
pronunciation activities as a tool for bottom-up listening practice.
Many of the skills needed to perceive words in the speech stream are
closely linked to the skills needed for accurate spoken production.
Practicing these skills in tandem gives learners an opportunity to
improve both their speaking and listening simultaneously. Information
gap activities like minimal pair Battleshipgive learners a chance to
practice their production and perception of challenging contrasts in a
low-stakes context. Other examples of pronunciation-based listening
activities include intensive and selective tasks in which learners are
given minimally contrastive options and asked to determine which they
hear in an audio sample.
In summary, the ability to process complex streams of speech
remains a challenge for L2 learners at all levels, particularly for
those who have academic interests in mind where accuracy is more
important than gist. In our view, when language teachers skip some of
these important bottom-up listening steps and instead focus on testing,
not teaching listening, they overlook some of the crucial phases that
are necessary to achieve more advanced levels of L2 listening
comprehension. Our goal is to reconceptualize our L2 listening practice
to be more holistic, so that the bottom-up skills are considered
necessary building blocks for a more complicated, nuanced listening
adventure.
References
Field, J. (2008). Listening in the language
classroom. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Press.
Goh, C. (2000). A cognitive perspective on language learners’
listening comprehension problems. System, 28,
55–75.
Jensen, E. D., & Vinther, T. (2003). Exact repetition
as input enhancement in second language acquisition. Language
Learning, 53, 373–428.
Matthews, J., & Cheng, J. (2015). Recognition of high
frequency words from speech as a predictor of L2 listening
comprehension. System, 52, 1–13.
Siegel, J. (2016). Listening vocabulary: Embracing forgotten
aural features. RELC Journal, 47(3), 377–386.
Jennifer A. Lacroix is a senior lecturer at Boston
University. She is currently working on a doctorate in language
education with a focus on L2 listening pedagogy. She is actively
involved with TESOL International Association and presents regularly at
conferences.
Abigail J. Castle is an ESL lecturer at the University
of Iowa. She holds an MA in linguistics with a TESOL focus. Her
professional interests include bottom-up listening pedagogy, building
active L2 vocabulary, and creating content-based modules for student
success. |