Despite many teachers agreeing that pronunciation training is
important for learners, it has long been neglected in language
classrooms (Murphy, 2014). Teachers report feeling uncertain about how
best to teach it and also often note that there is little classroom time
available for its instruction. Others believe that communicative
language teaching is incompatible with focusing on the form of
pronunciation, and so avoid it altogether (Isaacs, 2009). The net result
is that when pronunciation instruction does happen, it is often
incidental, primarily occurring in the context of particularly egregious
breakdowns in communication.
Given these limitations and learners’ need for massive amounts
of exposure and practice in order for more accurate pronunciation to
develop, computer-assisted pronunciation training (CAPT) seems like an
obvious complement to traditional classroom instruction. Not only can
CAPT help raise learners’ awareness of English pronunciation features,
but it also allows for the large-scale exposure and practice that is
necessary for improvement.
Unfortunately, relatively few pronunciation software
applications are available, and those that do exist are not always
evidence-based (Derwing & Munro, 2015). Good pronunciation
software should be capable of addressing individual differences in
learning. So, for example, applications should be customizable to the
specific needs of learners from particular first-language backgrounds.
They should also be capable of focusing learners’ attention on sounds
rather than only on words (Thomson & Derwing, 2016).
English Accent Coach
One such program is English Accent Coach (EAC; www.englishaccentcoach.com),
a free web-based application that allows learners to autonomously
develop their perception of English vowels and consonants. The name of
the website reflects terminology used by end-users who search for it,
rather than intending to imply that foreign accents are problematic. In
fact, the aim of this application is to improve speakers’
intelligibility, not global accent.
EAC follows the High Variability Phonetic Training paradigm
(HVPT). This technique is not as complex as its name makes it sound.
Essentially, HVPT refers to a type of perceptual training that presents
learners with training stimuli in numerous phonetic contexts, spoken by
different talkers. It can be roughly conceived of as minimal pairs on
steroids. Laboratory research has repeatedly demonstrated that this
technique is more effective than traditional practices, in which training stimuli are spoken by a single
talker, or in which training exercises focus on sounds in a limited
number of contexts (Thomson, 2011). Unlike a simple minimal pair approach, HVPT promotes
the extension of perceptual learning during training to new talkers and
to new items. HVPT has also been shown to promote transfer of
perceptual training to production (see Thomson, 2011, for a detailed
overview).
EAC takes HVPT out of the laboratory and makes it available to
learners on a large scale. Because the application is web-based, it can
be used at learners’ leisure, as long as they have access to a computer
with a high-speed internet connection, and a quiet space. EAC allows
learners to focus on individual areas of difficulty in perceiving
English vowels and consonants. Preset levels promote learning of target
sounds in increasingly complex contexts, from isolated syllables (many
nonsense syllables) to stressed syllables in disyllabic words. All training items are spoken by 30 talkers, to my awareness the largest number of talkers ever used in HVPT training. This degree of variability is intended to ensure that
learners will not repeatedly hear the same training token during a
single training session. During a typical session, learners hear tokens
containing target vowels or consonants and must click on the phonetic
symbol for the sound that they believe they heard (see Figure 1). After
each click, the game provides immediate auditory and visual feedback on
the accuracy of the user’s selection. The vowel game also includes a
switch, which allows users to click on color key words instead of
phonetic symbols (e.g., green contains the sound /i/,blue contains /u/).
Figure 1. Screen shot of English Accent Coach vowel training application
Research Findings
Research using EAC and a precursor application (see Thomson,
2011) has been very promising. Apart from helping to determine if such
training is effective, this research has also shed light on how second
language pronunciation develops. For example, training learners to
perceive 10 English vowels in one phonetic context (i.e., following /b/
and /p/) was found to result in only limited transfer of learning to the
same ten vowels following other consonants. While the positive impact
of training on the perception of vowels in the training context is
encouraging, these early results demonstrated that second language
pronunciation may not develop sound-by-sound, but rather
context-by-context.
In a large-scale follow-up study over a 2-month period
(comprising approximately 10-12 hours of combined training over 40
sessions) 15 learners were trained to recognize 10 English vowels. For
the first nine sessions, vowels were presented in isolated /h + vowel/
syllables. For the next 29 sessions, the same vowels were presented
after other English consonants. Finally, training returned to the
original /h + vowel/ context for sessions 39 and 40. During the first
nine sessions with the /h + vowel/ context, the learners’ recognition of
English vowels improved session-by-session. However, despite 29
intervening training sessions in other contexts (10–38), when the /h +
vowel/ context was reintroduced during session 39, no further
improvement in vowel recognition was found to have occurred. This can be
attributed to the fact that from sessions 10-38, learners only ever
heard the 10 English vowels in other phonetic contexts. At the same
time, whenever any other phonetic context was repeated (e.g., vowels
after /b/, /d/, and /g/, during sessions 10 and 17), vowel recognition
scores in those contexts also improved.
In another study (Thomson & Derwing, 2016), my
colleague and I investigated whether EAC training that presents 10
vowels in nonsense syllables leads to greater improvement in the
pronunciation of 70 target words containing those vowels, relative to
training that primarily presents vowels in the 70 target words. To
answer this question, 31 learners were randomly assigned to either the
phonetically-oriented group, the word-oriented group, or a control
group. An elicited imitation task was used to record the learners’
productions of the 70 target words, before and after training (i.e.,
they heard the words in the sentence, The next word
is _____ and had to repeat the word in the sentence, Now I say ______). We found that the phonetically
oriented training group significantly improved in their pronunciation of
the 70 target words, while the word-focused group—trained to perceive
vowels using recordings of the 70 target words—did not.
Another interesting findingfrom EAC studies is that age does not appear to limit
learning. Learners as old as 60 years of age have participated in EAC
research with encouraging results. While older learners tend to have the
lowest scores at the outset, EAC facilitates rapid improvement. In
fact, the oldest learner in the 2011 Thomson study, a 50-year-old
female, demonstrated one of the greatest improvements.
Using EAC to Inform and Complement Traditional Classroom Instruction
Results of EAC studies (e.g., Thomson, 2011; Thomson & Derwing, 2016) can be used to inform classroom pronunciation
instruction. For example, instead of assuming that using the minimal
pair ship/sheep will result in the learning of these
contrastive vowel sounds in all word pairs, teachers likely need to
present many more pairs in order for learning to occur (e.g., hit/heat, sip/seep, fit/feet). Teachers should also be sure to
incorporate some practice of target sounds in nonsense syllables,
because this appears to lead to larger gains than training that contains
only real words.
EAC cannot replace the teacher, but it provides an important
complement to any course of pronunciation instruction. Learners can
create their own user accounts, which will allow them and their teachers
to track progress over time. In addition to a progress tool, learners
can also download and print report cards for individual sessions. This
functionality allows teachers to assign EAC activities as homework and
gives learners the ability to demonstrate that specific training
sessions have been completed.
References
Derwing, T. M., & Munro, M. J. (2015). Pronunciation fundamentals: evidence-based perspectives for L2
teaching and research. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing
Company.
Murphy, J. (2014). Myth 7: Teacher training programs provide
adequate preparation in how to teach pronunciation, In L. Grant, L.
(Ed.). Pronunciation myths: Applying second language research
to classroom teaching. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan
Press.
Thomson, R. I. (2011). Computer assisted pronunciation training: Targeting second language vowel perception improves pronunciation. CALICO Journal,
28, 744-765.
Isaacs, T. (2009). Integrating form and meaning in L2
pronunciation instruction. TESL Canada Journal, 27,
1-12.
Thomson, R. I., & Derwing, T. M. (2016). Is phonemic
training using nonsense or real words more effective? In J. Levis, H.
Le., I. Lucic, E. Simpson, & S. Vo (Eds.). Proceedings
of the 7th Pronunciation in Second Language Learning and Teaching
Conference. Oct. 2015. (pp. 88-97). Ames, IA: Iowa State
University.
Ron Thomson is associate professor of applied
linguistics at Brock University. His research focuses on the development
of second language pronunciation and oral fluency. |