 |
Final consonants occurring at the end of English words are
difficult for English language learners (ELLs) to pronounce accurately;
furthermore, consonant clusters that occur word-finally seem
particularly challenging. According to universal language theories, most
world languages have a preference for a syllable that ends in a vowel.
Second, most languages greatly restrict the types and numbers of
consonants that can occur at the end of a syllable or word-finally
(Blevins, 1995). English is an unusual language in that it functions
contrary to these two language universals as any English consonant
(except /h/) may occur word-finally, and English word-final consonant
clusters may contain three or, more rarely, four consonants. Therefore,
because of this particular phonological markedness of English, many
groups of English language learners struggle to produce English
word-final consonant(s). Empirical studies conducted on native speakers
of East Asian languages, Spanish, and Brazilian Portuguese have
demonstrated that English language learners tend to favor one or two
ways of modifying English word-final consonant clusters based on
constraints of their native phonologies (Hancin-Bhatt & Bhatt,
1997; Kim, 2001; Tarone, 1980). In addition, language universals also
appear to be a factor in the systematic errors that English language
learners make in their interlanguage (Broselow, Chen, & Wang,
1998; Tarone, 1980).
Based on the existing research on East Asians’ pronunciation
difficulty of English word-final consonants, the present study sought to
investigate how consonant(s) that cannot occur in the native language
might be repaired or modified according to L1 constraints and
interlanguage. Korean was selected as a medium for examination based on
its simple syllable structure (consonant-vowel-consonant). Korean
restricts which consonants can occur syllable-finallyand word-finally;
further, it does not allow consonant clusters. The research questions
guiding the present study were as follows: Does the number of English
word-final consonant(s) prove to be a factor in the types of
modifications native Korean participants make? What environment
conditions the processes or repairs? What characterizes repair processes
according to English proficiency level?
Six native Koreans enrolled at an intensive English program
(IEP) at an American university participated in the present study. Two
participants were selected from each of the following levels (as
determined by the IEP placement test, which includes both an oral
interview and a listening task): beginning, intermediate, and advanced
English proficiency. Participants were individually interviewed by the
researcher once in an hour-long session. Participants completed three
speaking tasks: reading a list of 150 words, watching and retelling a
film clip by Disney Pixar, and answering questions in a personal
narrative. All participants were digitally recorded; the speech samples
were then uploaded onto a computer and phonetically analyzed by the
researcher using Praat waveform and spectrogram software to determine if
correct pronunciation was achieved or if errors of a phonetic nature
occurred to the target consonant(s). Data was coded for type of error,
number of final consonants, environment of the final consonant(s), and
level of participant. Chi-squares were run on pair-wise variables to
test for significant differences using SPSS.
The first research question addressed the types of
modifications and the number of word-final consonant(s) in the target
word. Modifications consisted of phonetic changes to the word-final
consonant(s). Repairs were coded as correct, deletion, epenthesis,
substitute manner, substitute place, substitute voice, or multiple
repairs. A segment was correct if it was produced correctly by the
participant. For deletion, a consonant was omitted;
“grab” became /græ/. For epenthesis a vowel was added
to the final consonant(s): “just” became /jus.tə/. For substitute manner the manner of articulation was
changed; a stop became a fricative in /greifs/ “grapes.” For substitute
place, the place of articulation changed as in /greiv/ “graze.” For substitute voice, obstruent voicing was changed;
“cab” was pronounced /cæp/. Finally, in multiple
repairs more than one of the aforementioned repairs occurred.
Table 1 displays the interaction
between number of consonants and type repairs. The results were
significant: χ²(12, n = 3,269) = 464.913, p < .001.
Table 1
The Number of Word-Final Consonants and Type of Repair Process Used by All Participants
Type of Repair Process |
Number of
Consonants |
Correct |
Deletion |
Epenthesis |
Substitute Manner |
Substitute Place |
Substitute
Voice |
Multiple Repairs |
Totals |
Single Consonant |
61.3%
(1,438) |
18.9%
(443) |
2.3%
(53) |
3.1%
(73) |
.6%
(13) |
13.2%
(308) |
.7%
(16) |
100%
(2,344) |
Two
Consonants |
38.0%
(328) |
26.8%
(231) |
10.2%
(88) |
3.6%
(31) |
.6%
(5) |
9.8%
(85) |
11.0%
(95) |
100%
(863) |
Three or more
Consonants |
22.6%
(14) |
32.3%
(20) |
21.0%
(13) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
1.6%
(1) |
22.6%
(14) |
100%
(62) |
Table 1 shows that as the number of coda consonants
increased, accuracy of production decreased. Overall correct production
was 54.5 percent. Correct production of a single word-final consonant
was 61.3 percent. The accuracy for two consonants fell to 38.0 percent.
Correct production of three consonants fell to 22.6 percent. Among the
repairs that were used to modify final consonants, deletion was the most
frequent repair, particularly for one and two consonants. However, in
the case of three consonants, multiple repairs and epenthesis were
frequently used as well. Furthermore, three consonants also appeared to
trigger an increase in multiple repairs.
The second research question addressed whether environment
could trigger a specific modification used by participants. The seven
repair types from Table 1 were again used in these environmental
analyses tables. Environment was divided into four categories based on
sonority: vowel, voiced obstruent, voiceless obstruent, and sonorant.
Vowel described a vowel immediately preceding the word-final consonant
as in “hat.” Voiced obstruent referred to the segment before the
epenthetic repair /gɪvə/ “give.” Voiceless obstruent was the segment
before the repair /fɪʃi/ “fish.” Sonorant was the segment before repair
in /drɪlə/ “drill.” Tables 2 to 4 display the interactions of one, two,
and three final consonants and the repairs that were most commonly used.
The results were significant: χ²(18, n = 2344) =
2017.088, p < .001.
Table 2
The Preceding Segment of One Consonant and Repair Process Used by All Participants
Type of Repair Process |
Preceding
Segment |
Correct |
Deletion |
Epenthesis |
Substitute
Manner |
Substitute Place |
Substitute Voice |
Multiple Repairs |
Totals |
Vowel |
62.8%
(1,438) |
19.3%
(443) |
.3%
(6) |
3.2%
(73) |
.6%
(13) |
13.4%
(308) |
.4%
(9) |
100%
(2,290) |
Voiced
Obstruent |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
95.7%
(22) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
4.3%
(1) |
100%
(23) |
Voiceless
Obstruent |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
84.0%
(21) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
16.0%
(4) |
100%
(25) |
Sonorant |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
66.7%
(4) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
33.3%
(2) |
100%
(6) |
Table 2 shows that in the case of one word-final consonant,
overall accuracy was 61.3 percent. In accurately produced tokens, a
vowel directly preceded the final consonant. The two most common repairs
to a single final consonant were deletion and substitution of voicing
(devoicing). In the cases of final obstruents, the numbers of epenthetic
repairs were much higher than those in the preceding vowel and sonorant
categories.
Table 3 shows production of two consonant clusters; the
accuracy fell to 38 percent for these segments. Deletion remained the
overall favored repair. The number of multiple repairs increased with
the addition of a second final consonant. Again, epenthetic repairs,
which were not favored overall, remained the preferred repair for
environments of preceding voiced and voiceless obstruents. The results
were significant: χ² (18, n = 863) = 879.094, p < .001.
Table 3
The Preceding Segment of Two Consonants and Repair Process Used by All Participants
Type of Repair Process |
Preceding
Segment |
Correct |
Deletion |
Epenthesis |
Substitute
Manner |
Substitute Place |
Substitute Voice |
Multiple Repairs |
Totals |
Vowel |
62.0%
(328) |
17.6%
(93) |
0%
(0) |
2.8%
(15) |
0%
(0) |
.8%
(4) |
16.8%
(89) |
100%
(529) |
Voiced
Obstruent |
0%
(0) |
15.2%
(5) |
69.7%
(23) |
3.0%
(1) |
0%
(0) |
3.0%
(1) |
9.1%
(3) |
100%
(33) |
Voiceless
Obstruent |
0%
(0) |
41.1%
(37) |
53.3%
(48) |
4.4%
(4) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
1.1%
(1) |
100%
(90) |
Sonorant |
0%
(0) |
45.5%
(96) |
8.1%
(17) |
5.2%
(11) |
2.4%
(5) |
37.6%
(80) |
.9%
(2) |
100%
(211) |
Table 4 shows production of three consonant clusters; the
accuracy fell to 22.6 percent. Deletion was again the favored repair,
accounting for an overall total of 32.3 percent of all utterances, which
exceeded the number of correct tokens. Multiple repairs were the second
most common repair, accounting for 22.6 percent of utterances.
Epenthesis accounted for 21.0 percent of all utterances. The results
were significant χ² (12, n = 62) = 57.987, p < .001.
Table 4
The Preceding Segment of Three Consonants and Repair Process Used by All Participants
Type of Repair Process |
Preceding
Segment |
Correct |
Deletion |
Epenthesis |
Substitute
Manner |
Substitute Place |
Substitute Voice |
Multiple Repairs |
Totals |
Vowel |
82.4%
(14) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
17.6%
(0) |
100%
(17) |
Voiced
Obstruent |
0%
(0) |
25.0%
(1) |
50.0%
(2) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
25.0%
(1) |
100%
(4) |
Voiceless
Obstruent |
0%
(0) |
43.8%
(7) |
43.8%
(7) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
12.5%
(2) |
100%
(16) |
Sonorant |
0%
(0) |
48.0%
(12) |
16.0%
(4) |
0%
(0) |
0%
(0) |
4.0%
(1) |
32.0%
(8) |
100%
(25) |
The third research question addressed whether English fluency
level had an influence on the type of repair used. The fluency levels of
the participants represented beginners, intermediates, or advanced
English language learners. Participant fluency levels were determined by
the IEP placement listening test and an oral interview. The seven
repair categories from the previous tables were used again. The
interaction of English fluency level and repair are shown in Table 5.
The results were significant χ² (12, n = 3269) =
48.712, p < .001.
Table 5
Participants’ Levels of English Proficiency and Type of Repair Process
Type of Repair Process |
Level of Proficiency |
Correct |
Deletion |
Epenthesis |
Substitute Manner |
Substitute Place |
Substitute
Voice |
Multiple Repairs |
Totals |
Beginner |
49.2%
(377) |
23.3%
(179) |
5.5%
(42) |
3.8%
(29) |
.5%
(4) |
13.6%
(104) |
4.2%
(32) |
100%
(767) |
Intermediate |
52.7%
(595) |
25.3%
(285) |
3.0%
(34) |
3.0%
(34) |
.6%
(7) |
11.1%
(125) |
4.3%
(48) |
100%
(1,128) |
Advanced |
58.8%
(808) |
16.7%
(230) |
5.7%
(78) |
3.0%
(41) |
.5%
(7) |
12.0%
(165) |
3.3%
(45) |
100%
(1,374) |
The results of Table 5 show that accuracy improved with
proficiency level. Furthermore, the more advanced students were better
able (or more willing) to communicate in English as evidenced by the
total utterances produced in the far right column. Among all
participants, deletion was the primary repair used in consonants and
clusters. Substitution of voice was a distant second repair among all
three proficiency levels. While overall errors did improve with
proficiency, the repairs that were used fluctuated between the
proficiency levels. Epenthesis in particular was used more frequently by
the advanced learners than by beginning and intermediate
learners.
The present findings that number of consonants and proficiency
level were factors in pronunciation accuracy corroborated with previous
studies on Korean (Kim, 2001; Kwon, 2007). However, one interesting
finding in this study was the larger number of epenthetic repairs used
by advanced learners. There appeared to be a trend that might be
attributable to the participants’ experiences in which epenthetic
repairs led to fewer misunderstandings than deletion repairs, a finding
that corroborates Abrahamsson (2003).
The present study demonstrated that deletion was a preferred
repair overall. Epenthesis was a secondary repair in the cases of
preceding voiced and voiceless obstruent environments. The preference
for epenthesis among obstruents could be a possible result of the
numerous English obstruents that either do not exist or are not
permitted word-finally in Korean. Korean linguists described both
epenthetic vowel insertion and deletion as common repairs to native
Korean illicit sequences and to loanwords from other languages (Ha,
Johnson, & Kuehn, 2009). These two repairs could be attributed
to native language influence and to universal preference for reduction
of word-final consonants as well. The final repair to emerge with
significance among the proficiency levels was that of devoicing. This
final repair was again inconclusive as to whether it was a factor of
native phonological processes or interlanguage preference for
universals. Korean stops are differentiated by aspiration rather than
voicing, so essentially all Korean stops are voiceless. A preference for
voiceless final consonant(s) by participants in this study could be
attributed to either, or perhaps both, native language and interlanguage
influences. Based on the results of the study, it was difficult to
differentiate the specific influences of native and interlanguage
universal preferences.
Potential areas of future investigation would be a further
division among final consonant environments to possibly include those
segments native to Korean and those that are nonnative to examine how
these two variables interact with data. A second area of potential
research might be to attempt to isolate some of the native Korean
phonetic factors and those that might be caused by a possible preference
for universals in interlanguage.
REFERENCES
Abrahamsson, N. (2003). Development and recoverability of L2
codas: A longitudinal study of Chinese-Swedish interphonology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,
25(3),313-349.
Blevins, J. (1995). The syllable in phonology theory. In J.
Goldsmith (Ed.), A Handbook of Phonological Theory,
(pp. 206-240). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Broselow, E., Chen, S. I., & Wang, C. (1998). The
emergence of the unmarked in second language phonology. Studies
in Second Language Acquisition, 20(2),261-280.
Ha, S., Johnson, C. J., & Kuehn, D. P., (2009).
Characteristics of Korean phonology: Review, tutorial, and case studies
of Korean children speaking English. Journal of Communication
Disorders, 42, 163-179.
Hancin-Bhatt, B., & Bhatt, R. M. (1997). Optimal L2
syllables. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 331-378.
Kim, H. K., (2001). The interlanguage phonology of Korean
learners of English: A computational implementation of optimality
theoretic constraints. Dissertation Abstracts international:
Section A. Humanities and Social Sciences, 61(11),
4362A-4363A.
Kwon, B. Y. (2007). Korean speakers’ production of English
consonant clusters: Articulatory and perceptual accounts. Dissertation Abstracts international: Section A. Humanities and
Social Sciences, 67(10), 3800.
Tarone, E. E. (1980). Some influences on the syllable structure
of interlanguage phonology, IRAL, 18(2),
139-152.
Elizabeth Conway is a PhD student at Rutgers
University. She is interested in segmental differences between languages
and the phonetic/phonological interface that present challenges to
English language learners’ pronunciation.
LinkedIn: http://www.linkedin.com/pub/elizabeth-conway/14/250/263 |