Mobile Version | Print-Friendly Version
TESOL Globe
April 2016
TESOL Globe
Forward to a Friend  |  RSS Feeds  |  Archives  |   Follow us on TwitterLike us on FacebookFollow us on LinkedIn
ADVERTISEMENT
Free TESOL Quarterly Article: "Second Language Comprehensibility Revisited: Investigating the Effects of Learner Background"
by Dustin Crowther, Pavel Trofimovich, Kazuya Saito, and Talia Isaacs

This article first appeared in TESOL Quarterly, Volume 49, Number 4, pgs. 814–837. Subscribers can access issues here. Only TESOL members may subscribe. To become a member of TESOL, please click here, and to purchase articles, please visit Wiley-Blackwell. © TESOL International Association.

Abstract
The current study investigated first language (L1) effects on listener judgment of comprehensibility and accentedness in second language (L2) speech. The participants were 45 university-level adult speakers of English from three L1 backgrounds (Chinese, Hindi, Farsi), performing a picture narrative task. Ten native English listeners used continuous sliding scales to evaluate the speakers' audio recordings for comprehensibility and accentedness as well as 10 linguistic variables drawn from the domains of pronunciation, fluency, lexis, grammar, and discourse. Comprehensibility was associated with several linguistic variables (segmentals, prosody, fluency, lexis, grammar), but accentedness was primarily linked to pronunciation (segmentals, word stress, intonation). The relative strength of these associations also varied as a function of the speakers' L1, especially for comprehensibility, with Chinese speakers influenced chiefly by pronunciation variables (segmental errors), Hindi speakers by lexicogrammar variables, and Farsi speakers showing no strong association with any linguistic variable. Results overall suggest that speakers' L1 plays an important role in listener judgments of L2 comprehensibility and that instructors aiming to promote L2 speakers' communicative success may need to expand their teaching targets beyond segmentals to include prosody-, fluency-, and lexicogrammar-based targets. 

With languages such as English, Spanish, Arabic, or Chinese becoming ever more prominent in international trade, education, and popular culture, particularly in communication among nonnative speakers, understanding various components of second language (L2) speaking ability emerges as an important goal for both language researchers and teachers. One key component of speaking ability is pronunciation, which has typically been discussed with reference to two broad constructs, namely, understanding and nativelikeness (see Derwing & Munro, 2009; Levis, 2005). Understanding embraces various aspects of speakers' ability to make themselves understood. Following common research and assessment practice, understanding is often measured as comprehensibility or listeners' perception of how easy or difficult it is for them to understand L2 speech, rated on 7- or 9-point scales. Nativelikeness, which broadly refers to speakers' ability to approximate speech patterns of the target-language community, is usually operationalized as a listener-based rating of accentedness, also using 7- or 9-point scales.

Although researchers have consistently underscored comprehensibility as a more realistic goal for ensuring communicative success, compared to accent reduction or nativelikeness (e.g., Derwing & Munro, 2009; Levis, 2005), there still remains a need to distinguish how various aspects of L2 speech (at the level of phonology, fluency, lexis, grammar, or discourse) feed into comprehensibility and how they impact accentedness. Previous research has shown that while accent is linked primarily to phonology- and fluency-based characteristics of L2 speech, comprehensibility is additionally linked to grammatical and lexical variables (e.g., Saito, Trofimovich, & Isaacs, 2014, in press; Trofimovich & Isaacs, 2012). However, despite the fact that several (if not most) theoretical perspectives in L2 pronunciation learning ascribe an important role to speakers' first language (L1) in determining the rate and ultimate success of learning (e.g., Eckman, 2004; Escudero & Boersma, 2004; Flege, 2003), previous research on comprehensibility and accent paid little attention to speakers' L1. In fact, most current evidence on L2 speech rating comes from studies that have either treated speakers of various L1s as a single group (e.g., Derwing, Munro, & Wiebe, 1998) or focused only on a single L1 group (e.g., Munro & Derwing, 1999). This limitation makes it unclear to what extent the linguistic variables that feed into comprehensibility and accentedness are specific to the speaker's L1. Therefore, the main objective of this study was to clarify the relationship between comprehensibility and accentedness, investigating the effect of speakers' L1 on listener perception of L2 comprehensibility and accentedness.

Disentangling Comprehensibility From Accent

For many L2 speakers and their teachers, the ideal ultimate learning goal is often to acquire the linguistic ability of a native speaker, characterized by native or near-native accent (Tokumoto & Shibata, 2011). However, adult L2 speakers rarely pass for native speakers (Bongaerts, van Summeren, Planken, & Schils, 1997), so accented L2 speech is generally seen as normal and often unavoidable, even for speakers who begin learning at an early age (Flege, Munro, & MacKay, 1995). Considering the difficulty of acquiring nativelike L2 speech, adopting a more realistic learning goal has been encouraged, with a particular focus on comprehensibility or ease of understanding (Derwing & Munro, 2009; Levis, 2005). Indeed, even a heavy L2 accent does not preclude speakers from being highly comprehensible (Munro & Derwing, 1999). A focus on comprehensibility also seems sensible from a practical perspective, given that the interlocutor's goal in most real-world contexts is to get his or her message across rather than to pass for a native speaker. Thus, to make informed decisions about future learning goals and to address these goals through instruction, L2 speakers and their teachers need to know which aspects of language contribute to comprehensible speech and which are tied to foreign accent.

Download the full article and references for free (PDF)

This article first appeared in TESOL Quarterly, 49, 814–837. For permission to use text from this article, please go to Wiley-Blackwell and click on "Request Permissions" under "Article Tools."
doi: 10.1002/tesq.203

Previous Article Next Article
Post a Comment
Share LinkedIn Twitter Facebook
 Rate This Article
Print This ArticleForward This Article
Table of Contents
TC Homepage
Fostering High-Leverage Family Engagement
Tips for Testing Speaking
Raising Awareness of English Varieties
Quick Tip: Teaching Idioms
Free TQ Article: Second Language Comprehensibility
Association News
Resources
Job Link
Young Learner ELA Teacher (female only); MiSK International Schools; Riyadh, Saudi Arabia

ESLTeacher (Summer Camp); Tamwood Camps; Boston, Massachusetts, Los Angeles and San Jose, California, USA



Want to post your open positions to Job Link? Click here.

To browse all of TESOL's job postings, check out the TESOL Career Center.

ADVERTISEMENT
TESOL Blogs
Check out these recent TESOL Blogs:

5 Tips for Enriched Vocabulary Presentation

Revolutionizing Education by Reshaping Narrative

10 Online Resources to Improve EL Literacy

TESOL-IATEFL Discussion: ESP Projects for Social Change

Teaching English to Digital Natives: Common Computer Concerns for ELLs

ADVERTISEMENT

TESOL Connections is the newsletter of TESOL International Association
Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages

Active TESOL members may read current and recent issues of TESOL Connections online at http://www.tesol.org/tc. Inclusion in TESOL Connections does not constitute an endorsement by TESOL.

For article guidelines: www.tesol.org/tc/submissions
For questions about TESOL Connections: tc@tesol.org
For questions about copyright or permission: permissions@tesol.org
For advertising: tesol@bluehouse.us

TESOL International Association
1925 Ballenger Avenue, Suite 550 Alexandria, VA 22314-6820 USA
Tel. +1 703.836.0774
Fax: +1 703.836.7864
E-mail: info@tesol.org (general information)
www.tesol.org