Children today are exposed to technology at a very young age,
with educational media becoming increasingly salient in the lives of
young children. In fact, preschoolers in the United States spend an
average of more than 2 hours on screen per day (Rideout, 2014), of which
the majority are purported to be educational (Common Sense Media,
2013). These educational programs have been designed to provide
preschoolers with early learning experiences to increase school
readiness and educational outcomes. While educational media has the
potential to serve as a powerful mechanism for vocabulary development
and oral language comprehension (Guernsey, Levine, Chiong, &
Stevens, 2012), these benefits may extend, especially, to the English
learner (EL) population. Still, with viewing habits exceeding 2 hours
per day in our youngest generations, two important questions emerge:
What are children watching, and how educational are these educational
programs?
Literature documents the importance of a child’s vocabulary
knowledge in the early years as predictors of comprehension in the
middle school years (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997). Research
also asserts that young children who enter school with limited oral
language comprehension are at risk for encountering difficulties in
early literacy, which includes vocabulary development (Cunningham
& Stanovich, 1997). Compounding this disparity, children from
families where English is not spoken in the home often come to school
with a limited English vocabulary (August & Shanahan, 2006;
Mancilla-Martinez & Lesaux, 2010; Yesil-Dagli, 2011). In fact,
among ELs, a limited second language vocabulary is the most pervasive
obstacle to second language reading comprehension (August &
Hakuta, 1997; Carlo et al., 2004).
Despite the multitude of evidence that supports the need for
vocabulary learning in the early childhood and elementary school years,
there is a dearth of explicit vocabulary instruction in most curricula
(Neuman & Dwyer, 2009). Research indicates that explicit
vocabulary instruction is particularly important for ELs who benefit
from added instructional support (Gersten & Baker, 2000; Gersten
& Geva, 2003; Moats, 2001). In 2013, Marulis and Neuman conducted a
metaanalysis to determine which pedagogical features were associated
with the greatest effects for vocabulary learning. Findings demonstrated
that exposing children to educational media supports was one of the
most effective approaches for enhancing word learning.
Current Study
In light of the potential impact that educational media may
have on vocabulary development in ELs, the current study sought to
systematically investigate screen-based pedagogical support in
children’s educational media. More specifically, this study aimed to
identify the factors associated with screen-based vocabulary teaching
through a content analysis of commercially available streamed
media.
Methods
A content analysis is foundational for operationalizing and
examining screen-based pedagogical support in commercial educational
media. Through a content analysis, I sought to (1) identify the features
of educational media that support vocabulary acquisition and
comprehension, and (2) examine how frequently these features were used
to promote vocabulary acquisition. Employing a content analysis on
educational streamed media from Netflix and Amazon Prime, as these are
commercially available for families of ELs, this study honed in on five
television programs specifically designed to support early literacy in
preschool ELs.
Highly ranked by Common Sense Media (2016), the content
analysis was conducted on five programs with ethnic minority main
characters:
- Dora the Explorer
- Go, Diego, Go!
- Handy Manny
- Maya and Miguel
- Ni-Hao, Kai Lan
Using a randomized sample of these streamed videos (N=50),
programs were systematically coded for vocabulary learning experiences
according to an iteratively developed codebook with 16
vocabulary-teaching strategies. Episodes were viewed twice with an
interrater reliability of 87.3% for identifying vocabulary learning
experiences, and 82.1% for identifying screen-based pedagogical supports
for vocabulary learning.
Findings
An appropriate amount of time is spent teaching vocabulary words in media.
From a total of 50 videos in the weighted sample, findings
revealed that an average show taught vocabulary words for 11.0% of the
total running time. In addition, an average of 6.42 words were taught
per episode. Although the number of words per episode are higher than
the recommended two to three new words at the preschool level, these
results are encouraging as they exhibit a reasonable proportion of time
devoted to vocabulary instruction, and demonstrate marked differences
when compared to a parallel study on all streamed educational media, which yielded a mere
3.9% of vocabulary teaching per program episode (Danielson, Wong,
Neuman, & Flynn, 2017).
Vocabulary teaching in media utilizes insufficient pedagogical strategies.
To understand how vocabulary words are taught in media, each
word was coded for screen-based pedagogical supports that facilitate
vocabulary acquisition. The most commonly used vocabulary supports in
educational media included the use of repetition of the target word
(41.6%), visual effects (40.7%), and the use of demonstrations to teach
the target word (32.3%). Interestingly, pedagogical supports that
provided children with an explicit definition were markedly lower
(5.6%). Taken together, the most salient strategies that specifically
benefit ELs vocabulary acquisition were found in commercially available
media, yet critical supports with demonstrated benefits, like the use of
explicit definitions, were lacking.
Vocabulary words in media are superficial.
Finally, in an effort to understand the quality ofvocabulary
words being taught in educational media, we examined vocabulary words
according to three distinct word lists. We found that a large portion of
words were identified as simple, representing 63.6% on the Dale-Chall
(Chall & Dale, 1995) word list. Triangulating these results,
77.6% of the words were labeled as simple and elementary in Biemiller’s
(2010) “Words Worth Teaching” word list, and only 1.9% of the words
appeared in Tier 2 of Beck, McKeown, & Kucan’s (2002) three-tier
framework, which are considered high-utility words. Taken as a whole,
results from the content analysis suggest that despite the extensive
amount of time that children watch educational media, vocabulary instruction is
lacking in proven pedagogical strategies that support word learning.
Furthermore, the vocabulary words being taught are simple and do not
equip EL preschoolers for school readiness.
Conclusion
Children’s vocabulary knowledge is critical for their immediate
and future academic success. However, children from homes where English
is not spoken face particular challenges in acquiring early
literacy-related skills in English, such as vocabulary and oral language
development (August & Shanahan, 2006), and these challenges may
have profound implications on the trajectory of their language and
reading development (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997). By exploring
the effects that educational media may offer as a support for EL
children’s vocabulary knowledge and comprehension, we hope to contribute
to the research that prepares ELs for success and school readiness. By
identifying the factors associated with screen-based pedagogical support
and determining the relative influence of these factors on educational
outcomes, this study provides a better understanding of how educational
media can be used to encourage vocabulary development for EL children
during the early childhood years.
References
August, D., & Hakuta, K. (Eds.). (1997). Improving schooling for language-minority children: A research
agenda. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (Eds.). (2006). Developing literacy in second- language learners: Report of the
National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and
Youth. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Beck, I., McKeown, M., & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing words to life. New York, NY:
Guilford.
Biemiller, A. (2010). Words worth teaching: Closing
the vocabulary gap. Columbus, OH: McGraw-Hill SRA.
Carlo, M. S., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C., Dressler,
C., Lippman, D. N....White, C. E. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing
the vocabulary needs of English language learners in bilingual and
mainstream class- rooms. Reading Research Quarterly,
39, 188–216.
Common Sense Media. (2013). Zero to eight: Children's
media use in America 2013. Washington, DC: Common Sense
Media.
Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early
reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability
10 years later. Developmental Psychology, 33(6),
934–945.
Chall, J. S., & Dale, E. (1995). Readability
revisited: The new Dale-Chall readability formula.
Northampton, MA: Brookline Books.
Danielson, K., Wong, K. M., Neuman, S. B, & Flynn, R.
(2017). Content analysis of
vocabulary and pedagogical supports in educational
media. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Gersten, R., & Baker, S. (2000). What we know about
effective instructional practices for English-language learners. Exceptional Children, 66, 454–470.
Gersten, R., & Geva, E. (2003). Teaching reading to
early language learners. Educational Leadership, 60,
44–49.
Guernsey, L., Levine, M. H., Chiong, C., & Stevens, M.
(2012). Pioneering literacy in the digital wild west:
Empowering parents and educators. New York, NY: Joan Ganz
Cooney Center.
Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2010).
Predictors of reading comprehension for struggling readers: The case of
Spanish speaking language minority learners. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 102(3), 701–711.
Marulis, L. M., & Neuman, S. B. (2013). How vocabulary
interventions affect young children at risk: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 6,
223–262.
Moats, L. (2001). Overcoming the language gap. American Educator, 25, 4–9.
Neuman, S. B., & Dwyer, J. (2009). Missing in action:
Vocabulary instruction in pre-K. The Reading Teacher,
62(5), 384–392.
Rideout, V. J. (2014). Learning at home: Families’
educational media use in America. New York, NY: The Joan Ganz
Cooney Center.
Yesil-Dagli, U. (2011). Predicting ELL students’ beginning
first grade English oral reading fluency from initial kindergarten
vocabulary, letter naming, and phonological awareness skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(1), 15–29.
Kevin M. Wong is a PhD candidate at New York
University in literacy education. His research interests include second
language vocabulary learning, educational media, and comparative
education. His publications have appeared in The Asia-Pacific
Education Researcher and Reconsidering
Development, with forthcoming publications in Reading
and Writing, English Language Teaching Journal and The International Journal of Bilingual Education and
Bilingualism. |