VDMIS Newsletter - August 2014 (Plain Text Version)
|
|||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
In this issue: |
Articles THE EFFECTIVENESS OF THREE STRATEGIES TO TEACH VOCABULARY
Vocabulary acquisition is the backbone of second language learning. If a student uses the correct words with the wrong grammar, he or she will probably still be able to convey the message (Folse, 2004). Therefore, students and teachers of ESL or EFL are constantly searching for the best ways to learn vocabulary faster and retain it in long-term memory. Definitions, pictures, and translations are three of the most common ways to present and practice unfamiliar vocabulary. Definitions in the target language are very popular in ESL teaching, pictures are widespread in children’s or beginners’ classes, and translation is heavily relied on in many EFL settings. The present study aimed to test and compare the effectiveness of these three techniques, with the assumption that all three of them would produce optimal learning effects if applied at the same time. Paivio’s Dual Coding Model Paivio’s (2006) dual coding theory suggests that nonverbal stimuli, such as pictures and images, support verbal memory, including bilingual memorization of vocabulary. The stimuli enter the brain’s processing systems along two parallel channels, which, however, may also compete with each other (see Figure 1): Figure 1. The two parallel channels of Paivio’s (2006) Dual Coding Model. Kroll’s Bilingual Model Kroll’s (1993) bilingual model represents the different ways in which less fluent and more fluent second language speakers access concepts through their native or second language. It can be seen from Figure 2 that more concepts are easily accessible through the native language, and this happens especially at low or intermediate levels of second language proficiency:
According to the models in Figures 1 and 2, images and using the native language can help students learn vocabulary and retain it in their long-term memory. This study examined how memory works in vocabulary acquisition, in particular, and whether definitions alone or when accompanied with images and translations better aid intermediate ESL learners in remembering English vocabulary. A quasi-experimental research design exposed the same class of 19 high-intermediate intensive English program students (Chinese, Arabic, and Japanese) in the United States to three different vocabulary treatments—the first explained and practiced vocabulary using definitions only; the second definitions and pictures; and the third definitions, pictures, and translations. The means and standard deviations of participants’ pretest, immediate posttest, and delayed posttest scores were compared to find out if a combination of definitions, pictures, and translations would be the most effective way to present and practice new vocabulary. Research Questions Are there significant differences in the effectiveness of teaching vocabulary using definitions only; definitions and pictures; or definitions, pictures, and translation? Are there significant differences in the short-term and long-term retention of vocabulary taught using definitions only; definitions and pictures; or definitions, pictures, and translation? Procedures Pretests Treatment Immediate Posttests Delayed posttests Data Analysis The scores of the nine tests (three pretests, three immediate posttests, and three delayed posttests) were tabulated for each of the 19 participants. Mean scores and standard deviations were calculated for each test. The scores were also entered in the statistical software program PASW, Predictive Analysis Software (formerly known as SPSS, Statistical Package for the Social Sciences) and a one-way repeated measures ANOVA, Analysis of Variance, as performed to see if there were significant differences among the different treatments and the timing of the tests. Results The nine tests that the students took before and after each of the three treatments (teaching 10 vocabulary items explained first only in definitions; then, teaching 10 different vocabulary items with definitions and pictures; and finally, teaching 10 more vocabulary items using definitions, pictures, and translations in matching activities) were scored. The mean scores and standard deviations for all tests and treatments are presented in Table 1and Figure 3. Table 1. Test Results
Figure 3. Test Results A repeated-measures one-way ANOVA was run in PSAW statistical software. The results were computed using alpha = 0.05. As for the treatment, there were no significant differences among the effectiveness of the three treatments (sig = 0.297) but for the second factor, time, there were significant differences among the scores on the pretests, immediate posttests, and the delayed posttests (sig = 0.037). Discussion The results of the tests for the first and second vocabulary sets show a pattern. Students got the highest scores in the immediate posttests and the lowest scores in the pretest. The scores for the delayed posttests were lower than the immediate posttests but higher than the pretests. This pattern was expected, because students should be able to remember more words immediately after the treatment. Two weeks after the treatment, students forgot some of the words learned, as they did not revise or do any practice with the words between the treatment and the delayed posttest. However, the test results of vocabulary list 3 show a different pattern. The students still got the best results for the immediate posttest, but did worse in the delayed posttest. The mean score of the delayed posttest was very low (0.47), much lower than the pretest (1.38). Many of the test papers were turned in blank. One student received two points, and nobody scored higher than that. In the pretest, three students were able to obtain three points, which was the highest score. As for the number of students who got zeros, there were five in the pretest but ten in the posttest. In general, the overall results contradicted the hypothesis that vocabulary is learned more effectively with images and translations. The students performed better in both posttests for word list 1, which was taught with definitions only. The students continued to do badly in the following tests, which show that the images and translations did not help them to learn the words. On the contrary, it appears that they even had adverse effects on students’ vocabulary learning. As Paivio’s (2006) dual coding model might suggest, perhaps the pictures and the definitions, which were both in print, competed for the students’ attention. Pictures may also be confusing, especially for more abstract words: for instance if a smiley face is used to illustrate the word happy, students might think that it means smile. The use of pictures, however, seemed to help improve long-term memory as the results of this particular immediate posttest and delayed posttest were almost exactly the same. In other words, the students did not learn more words with pictures and definitions than with definitions alone, but whatever they learned seemed to stay in long-term memory without any further practice. Therefore, pictures may be helpful only after the meaning of a new word has already been clarified in a different way, and then a supplementary image association may aid memory and long-term retention. On the other hand, when translations were added to the definitions and pictures, the students did the worst. One possible reason could be that they were matching words in their native language to images, as one student reported doing, completely leaving out the English vocabulary and definitions. Again, perhaps the three different techniques again competed for the students’ attention and cognitively overloaded them. Translation may be a good, efficient technique on its own, but not combined with definitions in the target language and pictures at the same time. Conclusion In conclusion, simpler and shorter explanations seemed to be the best formula for successful vocabulary acquisition. Certainly more research is needed in this most fundamental, indispensable, and to some extent neglected area of second and foreign language learning.
Folse, K. (2004). Vocabulary myths: Applying second language research to classroom teaching. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press. Kroll, J. F. (1993). Accessing conceptual representations for words in a second language. In R. Schreuder & B. Weltens (Eds.),The bilingual lexicon (pp. 54–81). Amsterdam/ Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Paivio, A. (2006). Mind and its evolution: A dual coding theoretical interpretation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Dr. Alice Chan has been teaching English as a second language for more than 10 years in Hong Kong and the United States of America. Her research interest is in idiom instruction. Dr. Maria Petkova has been learning and teaching English as a foreign and second language for more than 35 years in Bulgaria, the United Arab Emirates, and the United States of America. Her research interests focus primarily on vocabulary and pragmatic competence.
Appendix B |